RBSC

Collapse

Announcement

Collapse
No announcement yet.

Skin Bleaching in Jamaica

Collapse
X
 
  • Filter
  • Time
  • Show
Clear All
new posts

  • Skin Bleaching in Jamaica

    Skin Bleaching in Jamaica: Self-Esteem, Racial Self-Esteem, and Black Identity Transactions

    Christopher A. D. Charles
    Monroe College, New York

    Abstract
    This study examines the individual self-esteem (SE) and the racial self-esteem (RSE) of Black Jamaicans who bleach their skin and the expression of the bleachers’ identity transactions in their interaction with other Black Jamaicans about skin bleaching. It was hypothesized that the bleachers will have comparable SE to the non-bleachers, but lower RSE. It was also hypothesized that the bleachers will use their racial identity to buffer the self against the Black Jamaicans who treat them badly, and conversely, to bond and bridge with those who treat them well because they are bleaching their skin. It was found that the bleachers had lower mean SE than the non-bleachers, but the two groups had comparable mean RSE. These results suggest that there is a negative relationship between SE and skin bleaching, and that bleachers have a basic Black worldview and identity, so their racial self-esteem is comparable to that of non-bleachers. The majority of the bleachers buffered in threatening interactions about skin bleaching, and they bridged and bonded in non-threatening interactions about skin bleaching. The skin bleachers’ racial identity served multiple functions in their identity transactions with other Blacks who spoke to them about skin bleaching.

    Skin Bleaching in Jamaica: Self-Esteem, Racial Self-Esteem, and Black Identity Transactions

    Skin bleaching is the use of dermatological creams, cosmetic creams, and homemade products by some people to bleach the melanin from the skin. This practice is a phenomenon that occurs in many countries including Jamaica (Charles, 2003a; Hall, 1994, 1995a, 1995b, 1999). Medical studies have documented the effects of skin bleaching (see for example, Ajose, 2005; Bwomda, 2005; Ly, 2007; Petit, 2006), but there is a dearth of research on why the practice of skin bleaching occurs. The Social Science studies that exist are mainly theoretical approaches that posit low self-esteem, colonialism, and White oppression as the causes of skin bleaching (Charles, 2003a; Hall, 1994, 1995b, 1999; Mire, 2001).

    There is very little empirical research on skin bleaching in terms of global self-esteem, collective self-esteem, and the relationship of these to Black identity. Therefore, the occurrence of skin bleaching among some Black Jamaicans provides an excellent opportunity to deconstruct Black identity (Charles, 2003b). Black identity here refers to a Black person’s identification with his or her racial group. The purpose of this current study is to examine how the people who bleach their skin regard their racial group; how they conceptualize the self; and how they use their Black identity in identity transactions or expressions with other Blacks with whom they interact about skin bleaching.

    Skin colour in Jamaica
    Jamaicans use the term “browning” to refer to non-White peoples with light or fair skin complexion. Preference for lighter skin tones was shown in a few studies among different samples. Mohammed (2000) has argued that light skin complexion is still important in male-female relationships. Mulatto women in Jamaica (the product of miscegenation between Blacks and Whites during the colonial period) are now the browning in the contemporary culture. These brown-skinned women are still the objects of desire for many Black men. Two studies (Cramer & Anderson, 2003; Gopaul-McNicol, 1995) have also shown preference for lighter skin tones in Jamaican children who were asked to identify their preference for a black or a white doll. Cramer and Anderson (2003) concluded that there was favouritism for White skin in older rural children (mean age 11.4 years) compared to younger rural children (mean age 5.6 years). However, the older children in the urban area (mean age 10.9 years) showed a preference for black skin. The urban kindergarten children equally selected the black and white dolls when they were asked about their ideal self. The rural fifth/sixth grade children showed favouritism for white dolls compared to the rural kindergarten cohort. Rural and urban boys (as compared with girls) saw the white targets as “nice.” Gopaul-McNicol (1995) also found that the majority of a cohort of Black Jamaican preschool children preferred to play with a white doll. Gopaul-McNicol argued that the preference for the white doll was an indication of self hate and rejection of Black racial identity. Similarly, Miller (2001) showed a preference for lighter skin tones among high school adolescents of all the skin colour groups (black, brown and white) he studied in Jamaica and that satisfaction with their body image was based on its closeness to light skin complexion, which is the ideal.

    Skin bleaching
    Skin bleaching occurs among people of colour in North America, Africa, the Caribbean, Europe, and Asia (Charles, 2003a; Hall, 1999). Several reasons have been given for the practice. Hall (1995a, 1995b, 1999) has argued that skin bleaching occurs because the cultural domination of Blacks by Whites causes some Blacks to internalize the white skin as more valued and ideal. Chisholm (2002) in a newspaper article suggested that some Blacks bleach their skin because they want to become White. Mire (2001) in an analysis of internet skin bleaching advertisements found that the politics of colourism which gives social benefits and access to light-skinned over dark-skinned people drove skin bleaching.
    In the case of Jamaica, Anderson (2000) posited that the bleaching syndrome is a function of the need to improve physical appearance because of the dysfunctional preoccupation with body perfection. Shepherd (2000) has stated that skin bleaching occurs because of an identity crisis among people of African descent and the disrespect they experience. Hickling and Hutchinson (2000) have argued that the prevalence of skin bleaching products in the market place suggests that the attainment of whiteness is a symbol of social acceptance. Moving beyond the studies above, Charles (2003a, 2003b, 2004) found that the participants in Jamaica who bleach their skin and those who do not bleach had the same average self-esteem score. In another study, Charles (2005) found that the majority of skin bleachers argued that Jamaica should always have a Black Prime Minister. This finding suggests that the skin bleachers hold a basic Black worldview (which is Afrocentric but not in the strictest sense) although they have modified their black physicality.

    The majority of the skin bleaching research has looked at the relationship between self-hate and skin bleaching in the context of White oppression, but only a few studies have looked at the relationship between skin bleaching and identity. It is therefore important to build on our current understanding of skin bleaching by including in this present study, an evaluation of the racial identity and identity transactions as they relate to skin bleaching.

    Identity
    There are several means by which the identity of the individuals who are bleaching their skin can be understood. For instance, individuals construct their identities by narrating life stories of the self that integrate past and present experiences and the anticipated future that gives meaning, purpose, and coherence to their lives (McAdams, 1988). This sense of authorship to freely create one’s self is especially important in urban cultures (Strenger, 2003). Theoretical perspectives also add to the understanding of identity processes. Individuals also identify with groups based on some common attributes like religion, gender, sexual orientation, race, ethnicity, skin colour and occupation (Deaux, 2000). A person’s self-concept is comprised of his or her personal identity or unique personal attributes, and the knowledge of his or her membership in social groups. Groups strive to achieve positive social identity by engaging in intergroup comparisons that lead to ingroup favouritism and outgroup discrimination, which enhance group self-esteem and interest (Luhtanen & Crocker, 1992; Tajfel & Turner, 1986). The salience of one’s identity is an important factor in judging identity-related stimuli. Momentary identity salience results from identity primes, which trigger information that is related to one’s identity and the distinctiveness of the identity in the social environment (Forehand, Deshpande & Reed, 2002).

    The various theoretical perspectives above give a general understanding of the basic identity processes experienced by individuals who bleach their skin. The skin bleachers alteration of their black physicality is a part of their life story that can be captured in the skin bleachers discussion about their identity transactions. The skin bleachers manipulation of their skin colour may make their racial identity salient. Therefore, race and skin colour are important identity categories that Black Jamaicans who bleach their skin have to deal with when they are negotiating their identity.

    Black identity transactions
    Black identity, in the current article, refers to a Black person’s identification with his or her race. The attachment to one’s race will vary among individuals. Identity transaction implies that identity is expressed in social interaction and is a universal psychological process that is not unique to any particular identity category like culture, race, occupation, religion, gender, ethnicity, or nationality. The phrase “Black identity transactions” suggests that Blacks express their racial identity through social interaction with others. Some Blacks in the United States use their identity to negotiate their daily interactions through “buffering” or protecting the self against racism and discrimination, “bonding” with other members of the Black community, and “bridging” which is the use of their interpersonal skills in social interaction with Whites. Blacks may also engage in “code switching” by becoming less Black if the situation demands it, or by engaging in “individualism” where they highlight their unique personality characteristics and act in a race-neutral way (Strauss & Cross, 2005).
    In a two-week daily diary study of identity transactions/expressions, African-American college students buffered themselves in stressful race related situations in which they felt threatened, and they indicated on the emotion checklist that they felt surprised, nervous, and sad during the interaction. There was code switching in race related situations that generated very little stress, and they reported feeling enthusiastic, peppy, and happy. There was bridging with European-Americans when there was positive affect or curiosity, and the students felt relaxed and satisfied with the situation.

    The students bonded when many African-Americans were present and the situation had very little stress, or there was positive affect and they felt peaceful, aroused, enthusiastic, relaxed, and happy. The students acted as individuals when they held neutral feelings toward European-Americans and they felt peaceful, calm, satisfied, and quiet during the interaction. The college students also transacted their identity with other Blacks through buffering, bonding, bridging and individualism (Strauss & Cross, 2005).
    This current study seeks to understand identity transactions among Blacks in Jamaica who bleach their skin and those who do not bleach. We do not know how the individuals who bleach their skin in Jamaica interact with other Blacks who compliment them or verbally abuse them because they are bleaching their skin. The social interaction between Jamaicans who bleach their skin and those who do not provides an empirical opportunity to understand Black identity transactions within the Jamaican context. It is also important to understand how the altered physicality of the skin bleachers is related to their total self-concept.

    Self-esteem and collective self-esteem
    Self-esteem and collective self-esteem are related and both are important in social identity because they are important in the individual’s overall self-concept. Self-esteem falls in the personal identity domain of the self-concept, which deals with how the individual regards the self. Collective self-esteem falls in the group domain or the individual’s reference group orientation, which involves how the individual regards the groups of which he or she is a member. Self-esteem and collective self-esteem are related to psychological adjustment (Cross, 1991; Luhtanen & Crocker, 1992; Tajfel & Turner, 1986). Tapping into the self-esteem and racial self-esteem of the study participants who bleach their skin and those who do not bleach their skin allows us to determine if the skin bleachers are experiencing an identity crisis (Shepherd, 2000) and if they suffer from self-hate (Gopaul-McNicol, 1995).

    Self-esteem is defined as the individual’s overall negative or positive attitude toward the self. Persons who have high personal self-esteem think they are individuals of worth, while those with low personal self-esteem have contempt or hatred for the self (Rosenberg, 1979). Several studies using different self-esteem measures found no major difference in level of self-esteem between Blacks and Whites (Rosenberg & Simmons, 1971). Collective self-esteem is defined as the overall negative or positive attitude a group member holds toward one of his or her ingroups. Persons who have high collective self-esteem have a high regard for their social group, and those who have low collective self-esteem have little regard or positive attitude toward their social group (Luhtanen & Crocker, 1992).
    The evaluative component of the self is important because women with low levels of self-esteem who perceived discrimination experienced greater depression (Corning, 2002). Women who have lower levels of collective self-esteem experienced greater levels of anxiety, somatization, and depression, as their perception of discrimination increased (Corning, 2002). It was also found that self-esteem was not a good predictor of how racism was construed (Rahimi & Fisher, 2002).

    After experiencing threat, people with high self- esteem became independent and were rated by a dyad partner as unlikable while people with low self-esteem became interdependent after threat, and were rated as likable. Therefore, interpersonal appraisals are a function of the fact that the relationship of threat and self-esteem changes peoples’ focus on different aspects of the self (Vohs & Heatherton, 2001).
    Individuals who have a weak bond, an individualistic cultural heritage, or low collective self-esteem for their group are more likely than others to respond in a favourable way to threat when they hold a high position in an unsuccessful group than when they hold a low position in a successful group.

    Individuals with high regard for their group are more likely to use their group’s performance to evaluate themselves. Individuals with high collective self-esteem react more favourably when their group does well than when it does poorly (McFarland & Buehler, 1995). Ingroup members who have high collective self-esteem are more likely to derogate the outgroup when there is a threat to collective self-esteem. This derogation is done to enhance the ingroup and maintain a positive social identity (Luhtanen & Crocker, 1992; Tajfel & Turner, 1986). Individuals who are low in collective self-esteem are more likely to construe racism in an ambiguous situation that involves a member of the ingroup than those who possess high collective self-esteem. In addition, individuals with low collective self-esteem compared to those with high collective self-esteem are more likely to attribute a higher degree of racism to the same incident (Rahimi & Fisher, 2002). This brief review is illustrative rather than exhaustive, and suggests that the regard for one’s social group also serves to protect the social self. Since the skin bleachers are altering their skin, it is useful to know how they regard their racial group.

    The research discussed above suggests that personal identity (PI) and reference group orientation (RGO) are important parts of the self-concept and they are related, but not correlated. The majority of the participants who bleached their skin in the skin bleaching studies conducted in Jamaica had mean self-esteem scores comparable to the participants who were not bleaching. However, the participants who were bleaching may have had low racial self-esteem (Charles, 2003a, 2003b, 2004). The self-esteem of the Jamaicans who bleach their skin tells us nothing about how they regard their racial group (RGO). Therefore, the purpose of this current study is to understand how the skin bleachers regard their racial group, and how they regard the self and the functions of their “brown skin” identity in their daily identity transactions or expressions with other Blacks.

    It is important to see if: (a) the findings of the self-esteem and skin bleaching studies in Jamaica can be replicated using a larger sample; and (b) understand the relationship between skin bleaching and racial self-esteem to see if the skin bleachers are rejecting their race. It is therefore hypothesized that the individuals who bleach their skin compared with individuals who do not bleach their skin will have comparable self-esteem and lower racial self-esteem. Additionally, it looks at how Black Jamaicans transact their identity with each other. No study on this phenomenon was found in the Caribbean literature. It is hypothesized that the individuals who bleach their skin will use the modified Black self to buffer against the Black Jamaicans who treat them badly, and will bond and bridge with those who treat them well because they are bleaching their skin.


    Method

    Participants
    A convenience sample of 54 participants was selected. They were 28 females and 26 males. There were 31 participants (18 females and 13 males) in the bleaching group and 23 participants in the non-bleaching group (10 females and 13 males).

    Measures
    Demographics. Participants responded to items about education levels, age, community of residence and gender.
    Self-esteem. The participants also completed the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale (Rosenberg, 1979), which is a 10-item Likert scale that measures global self-esteem. An exemplar is “I take a positive attitude toward myself.” The response choices were: strongly agree, agree, disagree, and strongly disagree. In five of the items the response choices ranged from 0-3 and for the other five items the response choice ranged from 3-0 because they were reversed in valence. The minimum score was zero and the maximum score was 30. Higher scores indicated higher self-esteem (Rosenberg, 1979). The scale’s internal consistency among the study participants was fairly high (Cronbach’s α = .73).
    Racial self-esteem. Participants also completed the race-specific version of the Collective Self-Esteem Scale (Luhtanen &Crocker, 1992). There was no difference in the psychometric properties between the general scale and the race-specific scale. The race specific version is a 16-item Likert scale that measures how individuals regard the racial group to which they belong. There are four subscales: membership self-esteem which is the extent to which people believe they are good members of their reference groups; private collective self-esteem which is the extent to which people hold their social groups in high regard; public collective self-esteem which is the extent to which people feel that others hold their social groups in high regard; and importance to identity which is the extent to which people believe that their social groups are an important part of their sense of self. An exemplar of the items on the racial self-esteem scale is “The racial group I belong to is an important reflection of who I am.” The response choices were: strongly disagree, disagree, disagree somewhat, neutral, agree somewhat, agree, and strongly agree. The response choices ranged from 1-7. Answers to 8 of the 16 items were reversed scored. A tally was taken for each subscale and then each subscale score was divided by 4. The minimum possible score was 4 and the maximum score was 28. Higher scores indicated higher racial self-esteem (Crocker, Luhtanen, Blaine & Broadnax, 1994; Luhtanen &Crocker, 1992). The internal consistency for the scale among the study participants was .78 (Cronbach’s alpha).
    Qualitative interview guide. There were also questions asking the participants if they bleached their skin, why they did so, and what product they used for bleaching. The identity transaction questions stated, “Give me an example of how you respond to people who treat you in a good way because you are bleaching your skin” and “Give me an example of how you respond to people who treat you in a bad way because you are bleaching your skin.” These qualitative questions tapping into identity as expression in good (non-threatening) interactions or bad (threatening) interactions were informed by the exploratory daily diary study of Black identity transactions of African-American college students in the United States (Strauss & Cross, 2005).
    The reasons the participants gave for bleaching their skin in response to the qualitative interview guide were coded and grouped according to similarity. For example, responses dealing with removing facial pimples, spots, and blackheads were coded as “remove facial bumps”. Responses that suggested that bleaching was done to attract a girl, a boy, a man, or a woman were coded as “to attract a partner.” The responses were coded by the author and two independent coders. The intercoder agreement was .75, .77 and .79.
    The responses dealing with Black identity transactions in threatening and non-threatening interactions were coded as buffering, bonding, or bridging. A threatening interaction was one in which a participant perceived the interaction as a personal attack. Any statement or behaviour reported by the participants that was protective or defensive of the self in their interaction with other Blacks about skin bleaching was coded as “buffering”. Any statement or behaviour reported by the participants that suggested positive affect and very comfortable and very enjoyable interactions was coded as “bonding”. Any statement or behaviour reported by the participants that did not suggest affect or very comfortable and enjoyable interaction, but rather basic everyday social courtesy, was coded as “bridging”. Any statement or behaviour within each mode of transaction (buffering, bonding and bridging) that triggered this mode of expression was coded as a “strategy”. The responses were coded by the author and two independent coders who were unaware of the hypotheses. The intercoder agreement was .82, .85 and .80.

    Procedure
    The author collected the data in Jamaica. Participants were recruited by advertising the study in two high schools (after receiving the permission of the school principals) and in one inner city community. The high schools selected were a traditional (church-affiliated) boys’ high school in the Kingston Metropolitan Area, and a co-educational upgraded high school in St. Catherine. The inner city community selected was a low-income garrison community in South St. Andrew. The criterion for selection of the schools and the community was ease of access. Participants who were interested in the study sought out the investigator. The informed consent of the parents, the assent of the students, as well as the consent of the community residents were received before the study was conducted. The bleaching group was comprised of the participants who said they were bleaching their skin. The non-bleaching group was comprised of the participants who said they were not bleaching.
    After administration of the measures, qualitative interviews dealing with skin bleaching and black identity transactions were conducted. The author addressed concerns and questions of the participants after the data collection process. The average length of each interview was 30 minutes. The interviews in the high schools took place at the guidance counsellor’s office, and the interviews in the community took place at the community centre.

    Data analysis
    The quantitative data were analyzed by t-test to determine the difference between self-esteem and racial self-esteem among participants who bleached and those who did not bleach. The qualitative data of the study were processed and analyzed using content analysis (Miles & Huberman, 1994; Denzin and Lincoln, 1998; Massey, Cameron, Ouellette & Fine, 1998). The interview transcripts were read in an open-ended way, and the emerging themes were documented. The data were further engaged in two ways. First, nested meanings and sub-codes within the documented data codes were sought. Second, comprehensive themes were searched for, as grouping elements for the coded data. The comprehensive themes were then used to construct an organizing framework to recognize analytical patterns of convergence and divergence within the data that gives cultural meaning and social relevance to the practice of skin bleaching.


    Results

    For the total sample the mean for self-esteem was 22.5 (SD = 4.48) and 20.8 (SD = 3.51) for collective self-esteem. The mean age of the participants was 17.6 years (SD = 3.3). The participants ranged in age from 13-33 years. Participants who bleached their skin had a significantly lower mean self-esteem score (M = 21.3) than the participants who did not bleach their skin (M = 24.1), t(52) = -2.31, p < 0.05. There was no significant difference in collective self-esteem between the bleaching group (M = 21.1) and the comparison group (M = 20.4), t(52) = 0.66, p > 0.05. A t-test was also done for the collective self-esteem subscales. The participants who bleached and those who did not had comparable mean scores on the membership self-esteem, the private collective self-esteem, and the public collective self-esteem subscales. The participants who bleached had a significantly higher mean score (M = 5.10) on the importance to identity subscale than those who did not bleach (M = 4.4), t(52) = 1.93, p < 0.05.
    Table 1 outlines the nature of the interaction (good and bad) between the skin bleachers and other Blacks, the mode of identity transaction (buffering, bridging and bonding), the strategies used in each mode of transaction and the frequency of responses. It was found that the majority in the bleaching group buffered themselves (Table 1) by ignoring those who treated them badly. The exemplar was “I don’t pay them any mind because at the end of the day they would like to bleach but some can’t afford it because it is expensive.” One participant laughed, and the exemplar was: “How I respond is to just laugh.” Other participants cursed during the bad interaction and the exemplar was “If they say ‘you ugly and you a bleach,’ I say it not stopping me from eating and I don’t feel anyway. I [also] say to them ugliness a sell. I say my ugliness pay. I pay to use things on my face.”
    Some of the participants in the bleaching group bridged (Table 1) with those who treated them in a good way by smiling. The exemplar was “I just smile;” For flaunting the exemplar was “I just model,” and for talking to them the exemplar was “I talk to them.” The participants bridged by saying “Thanks”. The exemplar was “When they say I look nice on the street, I say thank you.” Some bonded with those who treated them nicely, and the exemplar was “I deal with them on a good level like friends.” Some of the participants responded with more than one transaction. The exemplar was “I talk with them (bridging), and I treat them good because they treat me good (bonding).” There were no differences in the identity transactions based on age, gender, or social class. The participants’ community of residence was used to determine their social class.
    The participants in the skin bleaching group said they bleached their skin for several reasons such as their complexion was too dark, they bleached to remove facial pimples, to look beautiful and attract intimate partners among other reasons (Table 2). The participants reported using several products. These products were Symba Cream, Lexus Cream, Raw Nadinola, Neoprosone Gel, IKB Cream, Idole Soap, Ultra Cream, Ambi, and Cake Soap, similar to the findings in Charles (2003a, 2004). In the present study, the bleachers also reported using Bio Claire, Michael Jackson, Volume 40, Hotmovate Gel, Blue Soap, Revlon, Black and White Cream, Carrotis Cream, Mercury Top Cream, toothpaste, Extra Clear, and Immediate Clear. The majority of participants in the research group said they bought these bleaching products in shops, stores, supermarkets, and pharmacies. Downtown Kingston was one of the popular places where the products were bought. One participant got the creams from the United States. Two of the participants said they got the creams from friends and three said they got them from their mothers.


    Discussion

    The purpose of the current study was to understand how Jamaican Blacks who bleach the melanin from their skin regard their racial group vis-à-vis their regard for the self. It also investigated the functions of their altered physicality in their daily identity transactions with other Blacks who complimented or verbally abused them about the bleaching of their skin.
    Contrary to other studies (Charles, 2003a, 2004) participants who did not bleach their skin had higher individual self-esteem than those who did. This finding does not necessarily suggest that low individual self-esteem (a form of self-hate) causes skin bleaching, and further research is needed to demonstrate such causality. Since the skin bleachers were lower on self-esteem (a PI variable), and higher on racial self-esteem (a RGO variable), this means that in terms of the skin bleachers’ self-concept, self-esteem is not necessarily an indication of how they feel about their race, and vice versa.
    The lack of significant difference in racial self esteem between bleachers and non-bleachers suggests that bleachers see themselves as representatives of their racial group, and have a basic regard for their race. The skin bleachers high racial self-esteem also suggests that race is an important social identity category that gives meaning, coherence, and purpose to the lives of the skin bleachers. The salience of race in their RGO should therefore be used to judge race-related stimuli. Also, when the skin bleachers find themselves in contexts that make race salient, these contexts will trigger their racial identity. The skin bleachers identification with the Black race suggests that they will display ingroup favouritism toward Blacks to enhance group interest and esteem, and will display outgroup discrimination toward non-Blacks. This does not suggest that there is only one kind of Black identity or experience of blackness. Black identity in Jamaica is differentiated because within the Black group, higher status is accorded to progressively lighter shades of black. Additionally, some persons have higher regard for darker skin tones than others, increasing the complexity of the identifications.
    The participants who bleached their skin identified themselves as Blacks, but they altered their physicality for a higher/lighter shade of skin that gave them higher social status. In other words, the participants in the bleaching group identify with blackness, but not in the strict Afrocentric sense. These participants embrace their version of Black identity because they are identifying with Blacks who have “brown skin” - a common identity attribute. The skin bleachers viewed brown skin tone as a lighter shade of black. This may be one possible reason the participants who bleached their skin had a mean racial self-esteem score comparable to that of participants in the non-bleaching group.
    The bleachers had a higher mean score on the importance to identity subscale of the racial self-esteem scale than the non-bleachers. This finding suggests that the skin bleachers to a greater extent believe that their racial group is an important part of their self-concept compared with the non-bleachers. There are at least two possible explanations for this. Given that skin bleachers’ racial identity should give coherence, meaning, and purpose to their lives, one possible explanation is that the bleachers are altering their physicality for a lighter shade of black within their racial group so their Black identity becomes salient for them. In other words, these participants are preoccupied with their racial identity, which is expressed in the manipulation of their skin colour. However, alternatively, high importance to identity may be a defensive response to mask the low regard in which these participants hold their racial group. Therefore, these participants reported positively on the items that indicated their racial group was an important part of their self-concept.
    The bleachers reported reasons for altering their black physicality which suggest that they are making use of the perceived benefits of colourism, specifically the perceived social advantages of having light skin tone in Jamaica. The skin bleachers believe that skin bleaching makes them beautiful and attractive to their partners, unlike their pre-bleaching physicality which was too dark and therefore socially unacceptable. The skin bleachers with their low self esteem are more likely to be influenced by the colourized norms in the society. It is possible that the skin bleachers respond to these social norms in order to feel good about self.
    Whenever the participants experienced criticisms about skin bleaching in their identity transactions with other Blacks, they defended their sense of self. Although the skin bleachers were not asked about how they felt in the threatening interactions with other Blacks that triggered buffering, some of them reported a range of emotions that included being calm, upset, or humiliation. Similar emotions were reported by the college students in the identity transaction study in the United States (Strauss & Cross, 2005). The participants in the bleaching group responded positively when they were interacting with persons in non-threatening identity transactions. In addition to the support from some strangers, some of the participants reported getting the bleaching creams from their friends and their mothers. This provision of the products highlights the social support the skin bleachers receive from important members of their social group which legitimizes the practice of skin bleaching. The skin bleachers reported using a range of skin bleaching products, which suggests that these products are widely available in Jamaica—a fact that supports the social practice of skin bleaching. The participants who are bleaching their skin can find solace in relating to the persons who treat them well because participants reported experiencing positive emotions they defined as “happy” and “feel good” in the non-threatening identity transactions. These findings in Jamaica corroborate the findings of the exploratory daily diary study of African-American college students in the United States in terms of the relationship between the modes of identity transactions and emotions (Strauss & Cross, 2005).
    The skin bleachers’ self-reports about the threatening and non-threatening identity transactions in relation to skin bleaching are a part of their narrated or storied self. The skin bleachers integrate these narratives of their experiences and interactions about their altered physicality with the past experiences about their black physicality and the anticipated future benefits of having brown skin. The skin bleachers incorporate these narratives in their sense of self to make sense of who they are as Blacks in the Jamaican social environment. The threatening interactions make the skin bleachers’ brown skin identity salient because they have to defend the altered physicality. The non-threatening interactions also make the skin bleachers’ brown skin identity salient by validating it.
    There is a need for further research in order to understand the range of reasons for skin bleaching and its relationship with self-esteem and racial self-esteem, and how skin bleaching is related to other forms of body modification. In addition, it is also important to understand how individuals who bleach their skin transact their Black identity daily (over time) with other Blacks, rather than at a particular time with the use of self reports. Researchers in the future should also examine the relationship between self-esteem, racial self-esteem, and the modes of Black identity transactions. It is also important to research the range of emotions that are associated with Black identity transactions in Jamaica.
    The contribution of this current study to the field of psychology is that it has moved beyond the earlier studies of self-esteem and has tapped into the racial self-esteem of the skin bleachers. This study has also revealed how skin bleachers express their identity with other Blacks who engage them about skin bleaching and the emotions they experience in their daily interactions. One limitation of this article is the small convenience sample. Therefore, the results should not be generalized to Jamaican society. Additionally, the self reports provide a particular type of data, where the reasons given for bleaching may be perceptions rather than actual. Similarly, the behaviour of the bleachers may be due to reasons of which they are actually unaware. In addition, there is the possibility of a social desirability bias influencing the participants in that they endorse their racial group in order to avoid controversy and to obtain the approval of others.
    Winning means you're willing to go longer, work harder, and give more than anyone else - Vince Lombardi
Working...
X